An internet resource developed by
Christopher D. Green
York University, Toronto, Ontario
ISSN 1492-3713
(Return to index)
Introduction The word 'Volkerpsychologie' (folk psychology) is a new compound
in our [the German] language. It dates back scarcely farther than to about
the middle of the nineteenth century. In the literature of this period,
however, it appeared with two essentially different meanings. On the one
hand, the term 'folk psychology' was applied to investigations concerning
the relations which the intellectual, moral, and other mental characteristics
of peoples sustain to one another, as well as to studies concerning the
influence of these characteristics upon the spirit of politics, art, and
literature. The aim of this work was a characterization of peoples, and
its greatest emphasis was placed on those cultural peoples whose civilization
is of particular importance to us -- the French, English, Germans, Americans,
etc. These were the questions of folk psychology that claimed attention
during that period, particularly, to which literary history, has given
the name "young Germany." The clever essays of Karl Hillebrand on Zeiten,
Volker und Menschen (collected in eight volumes, 1885 ff.) are a good
recent example of this sort of investigation. We may say at the outset
that the present work follows a radically different direction from that
pursued by these first studies in folk psychology.
Practically coincident with the appearance of these earliest studies,
however, was a radically different use of the term 'folk psychology.' The
mental sciences began to realize the need of a psychological basis; where
a serviceable psychology did not exist, they felt it necessary to establish
an independent psychology [p. 2] foundation for their work. It was particularly
in connection with the problems of philology and mythology, and at about
the middle of the century, that the idea gradually arose of combining into
a unified whole the various results concerning the mental development of
man as severally viewed by language, religion, and custom. A philosopher
and a philologist, Lazarus and Steinthal, may claim credit for the service
of having introduced the term 'folk psychology' to designate this new field
of knowledge. All phenomena with which mental sciences deal are, indeed,
creations of the social community. Language, for example, is not the accidental
discovery of an individual; it is the product of peoples, and, generally
speaking, there are as many different languages as there are originally
distinct peoples. The same is true of the beginnings of art, of mythology,
and of custom. The natural religions, as they were at one time called,
such as the religions of Greece, Rome, and the Germanic peoples, are, in
truth, folk religions; each of them is the possession of a folk community,
not, of course, in all details, but in general outline. To us this fact
has come to appear somewhat strange, because in our age these universal
mental creations have already long transcended the limits of a single people.
Though this is true, it does not imply that the folk community is not really
the original source of these mental creations. Now, in the works of Lazarus
and Steinthal and in the Zeitschrift fur Volkerpsychologie und Sprachwissenschaft
edited by them and appearing in twenty volumes from 1860 on, the conception
had not as yet, it is true, received the precise definition that we must
give it to-day. Nevertheless, a beginning was made, and the new venture
was successfully launched along several different lines. Some uncertainty
still prevailed, especially with regard to the relation of these studies
to philosophy, and as to the method which psychology must follow when thus
carried over into a new field. It was only gradually, as the psychological
point of view gained ground in the special fields of research, that this
condition was improved. To-day, doubtless, folk psychology may be [p. 3]
regarded as a branch of psychology concerning whose justification and problem
there can no longer be dispute. Its problem relates to those mental products
which are created by a community of human life and are, therefore, inexplicable
in terms merely of individual consciousness, since they presuppose the
reciprocal action of many. This will be for us the criterion of that which
belongs to the consideration of folk psychology. A language can never be
created by an individual. True, individuals have invented Esperanto and
other artificial languages. Unless, however, language had already existed,
these inventions would have been impossible. Moreover, none of these languages
has been able to maintain itself, and most of them owe their existence
solely to elements borrowed from natural languages. How, again, could a
religion have been created by an individual? There have, indeed, been religions
whose founders were individual men: for example, Christianity, Buddhism,
and Islamism. But all these religions rest on earlier foundations ;they
are elaborations of religious motives arising within particular folk communities.
Thus, then, in the analysis of the higher mental processes, folk' psychology
is an indispensable supplement to the psychology of individual consciousness.
Indeed, in the case of some questions the latter already finds itself obliged
to fall back on the principles of folk psychology. Nevertheless, it must
not be forgotten that just as there can be no folk community apart from
individuals who enter into reciprocal relations within it, so also does
folk psychology, in turn, presuppose individual psychology, or, as it is
usually called, general psychology. The former, however, is an important
supplement to the latter, providing principles for the interpretation of
the more complicated processes of individual consciousness. It is true
that the attempt has frequently been made to investigate the complex functions
of thought on the basis of mere introspection. These attempts, however,
have always been unsuccessful. Individual consciousness is wholly incapable
of giving us a history of the development of human thought, for it is conditioned
by an earlier history concerning which it cannot of itself give us any
knowledge. [p. 4] For this reason we must also reject the notion that child
psychology can solve these ultimate problems of psychogenesis. Among cultural
peoples, the child is surrounded by influences inseparable from the processes
that arise spontaneously within its own consciousness. Folk psychology,
however, in its investigation of the various stages of mental development
still exhibited by mankind, leads us along the path of a true psychogenesis.
It reveals well-defined primitive conditions, with transitions leading
through an almost continuous series of intermediate steps to the more developed
and higher civilizations. Thus, folk psychology is, in an important sense
of the word, genetic psychology.
In view of the general nature of the task of the science, objection
has sometimes been raised to its being called folk psychology. For, the
study is concerned, not merely with peoples but also with more restricted,
as well as with more comprehensive, social groups. Family, group, tribe,
and local community, for example, are more restricted associations; on
the other hand, it is to the union and reciprocal activity of a number
of peoples that the highest mental values and attainments owe their origin,
so that, in this case, folk psychology really becomes a psychology of mankind.
But it is self-evident that, if it is not to fade into indefiniteness,
a term such as 'folk psychology' must be formulated with reference to the
most important conception with which it has to deal. Moreover, scarcely
any of the proposed emendations are practicable. 'Gemeinschaftpsychologie'
(community psychology) may easily give rise to the misconception that we
are concerned primarily with such communities as differ from the folk community;
'Sozialpsychologie' (social psychology) at once reminds us of modern
sociology, which, even in its psychological phases, usually deals exclusively
with questions of modern cultural life. In an account of the total development
of mental life, however -- and this is the decisive consideration -- the
'folk' is the most important collective concept and the one with which
all others are associated. The 'folk' embraces families, classes, clans,
and groups. These [p. 5] various communities are not excluded from the
concept 'folk,' but are included within it. The term 'folk psychology'
singles out precisely the folk as the decisive factor underlying the fundamental
creations of the community.
When this point of view is taken, the question, of course, arises whether
the problem thus assigned to folk psychology is not already being solved
by ethnology, the science of peoples, or whether it ought not to be so
solved. But it must be borne in mind that the greatly enlarged scope of
modern ethnology, together with the increased number and the deepened character
of its problems, necessarily precludes such a psychological investigation
as falls to the task of folk psychology. I may here be allowed to refer
to one who, perhaps more than any other recent geographer, has called attention
to this extension of ethnological problems -- Friedrich Ratzel. In his
treatise on anthropography and in a number of scattered essays on the cultural
creations of peoples, Ratzel has shown that ethnology must not only account
for the characteristics and the habitats of peoples, but must also investigate
how peoples originated and how they attained their present physical and
mental status. Ethnology is the science of the origin of peoples, of their
characteristics, and of their distribution over the earth. In this set
of problems, psychological traits receive a relatively subordinate place.
Apparently insignificant art products and their modifications may be of
high importance in the determination of former migrations, fusions, or
transferences. It is in this way that ethnology has been of valuable service
to history, particularly in connection with prehistoric man. The central
problem of ethnology concerns not only the present condition of peoples,
but the way in which they originated, changed, and became differentiated.
Folk psychology must be based on the results of ethnology; its own psychological
interest, however, inclines it to the problem of mental development. Though
of diverse origins, peoples may nevertheless belong to the same group as
regards the mental level to which they have attained. Conversely [p. 6]
peoples who are ethnologically related may, psychologically speaking, represent
very different stages of mental culture. The ethnologist, for example,
regards the Magyars and the Ostiaks of Obi as peoples of like origin. Psychologically,
they belong to different groups: the one is a cultural people, the other
is still relatively primitive. To the folk psychologist, however, 'primitive'
always means the psychologically primitive -- not that which the ethnologist
regards as original from the point of view of the genealogy of peoples.
Thus, folk psychology draws upon ethnology, while the latter, in turn,
must invoke the aid of the former in investigating mental characteristics.
The problems of the two sciences, however, are fundamentally different.
In fulfilling its task, folk psychology may pursue different methods.
The course that first suggests itself is to single out one important phenomenon
of community life after another, and to trace its development after the
usual pattern of general psychology in its analysis of individual consciousness.
For example, an attempt is made to trace the psychological development
of language by the aid of the facts of linguistic history. This psychology
of language is then followed by a study of the development of art, from
its beginnings among primitive races down to its early manifestations among
cultural peoples, at which point its description is taken up by the history
of art. Myth and religion are similarly investigated as regards the development
of their characteristics, their reciprocal relations, etc. This is a method
which considers in longitudinal sections, as it were, the total course
of the development described by folk psychology. For a somewhat intensive
analysis this is the most direct mode of procedure. But it has the objection
of severing mental development into a number of separate phases, whereas
in reality these are in constant interrelation. Indeed, the various mental
expressions, particularly in their earlier stages, are so intertwined that
they are scarcely separable from one another. Language is influenced by
myth, art is a factor in myth development, and customs and usages are everywhere
sustained by mythological conception. [p. 7] But there is also a second
path of investigation, and it is this which the present work adopts. It
consists -- to retain the image used above -- in taking transverse instead
of longitudinal sections, that is, in regarding the main stages of the
development with which folk psychology is concerned in their sequence,
and each in the total interconnection of its phenomena. Our first task;
then, would be the investigation of primitive man. We must seek
a psychological explanation of the thought, belief, and action of primitive
man on the basis of the facts supplied by ethnology. As we proceed to more
advanced stages, difficulties may, of course, arise with regard to the
delimitation of the various periods; indeed; it will scarcely be possible
to avoid a certain arbitrariness, inasmuch as the processes are continuous.
The life of the individual person also does not fall into sharply distinct
periods. Just as childhood, youth, and manhood are stages in a continuous
growth, so also are the various eras in the development of peoples. Yet
there are certain ideas, emotions, and springs of action about which the
various phenomena group themselves. It is these that we must single out
ii the content of folk psychology is to be classified, with any measure
of satisfaction, according to periods. Moreover, it should be particularly
noticed that, in starting our discussion with primitive man, as we naturally
must, the term 'primitive' is to be taken relatively, as representing the
lowest grade of culture, particularly of mental culture. There is no specific
ethnological characteristic that distinguishes this primitive stage from
those that are more advanced; it is only by reference to a number of psychological
traits, such as are indicative of the typically original, that we may determine
that which is primitive. Bearing: in mind this fact, we must first describe
the external traits of primitive culture, and then consider the psychological
factors of primitive life.
Of the second period in the development of civilization, we may safely
say that in many respects it represents a newly discovered world. Historical
accounts have nothing to say concerning it. Recent ethnology alone has
disclosed [p. 8] the phenomena here in question, having come upon them
in widely different parts of the earth. This period we will call the totemic
age. The very name indicates that we are concerned with the discovery
of a submerged world. The word 'totem,' borrowed from a distant American
tongue, proves by its very origin that our own cultural languages of Europe
do not possess any word even approximately adequate to designate the peculiar
character of this period. If we would define the concept of totemism as
briefly as possible, it might perhaps be said to represent a circle of
ideas within which the relation of animal to man is the reverse of that
which obtains in present-day culture. In the totemic age, man does not
have dominion over the animal, but the animal rules man. Its deeds and
activities arouse wonder, fear, and adoration. The souls of the dead dwell
within it; it thus becomes the ancestor of man. Its flesh is prohibited
to the members of the group called by its name, or, conversely, on ceremonial
occasions, the eating of the totem-animal may become a sanctifying cult
activity. No less does the totemic idea affect the organization of society,
tribal division, and the forms of marriage and family. Yet the elements
that reach over from the thought-world of this period into later times
are but scanty fragments. Such, for example, are the sacred animals of
the Babylonians, Egyptians, and other ancient cultural peoples, the prophetic
significance attached to the qualities or acts of animals, and other magical
ideas connected with particular animals.
Totemic culture is succeeded -- through gradual transitions -- by a
third period, which we will call the age of heroes and gods. Initial
steps towards the latter were already taken during the preceding period,
in the development of a rulership of individuals within the tribal organization.
This rulership, at first only temporary in character, gradually becomes
permanent. The position of the chieftain, which was of only minor importance
in the totemic age, gains in power when the tribal community, under the
pressure of struggles with hostile tribes, assumes a military organization.
Society thus develops into the State. War, as also [p. 9] the guidance
of the State in times of peace, calls out men who tower far above the stature
of the old chieftains, and who, at the same time, are sharply distinguished
from one another through qualities that stamp them as typical personalities.
In place of the eldest of the clan and the tribal chieftain of the totemic
period, this new age gives rise to the hero. The totemic age possesses
only fabulous narratives; these are credited myths dealing, not infrequently,
with animal ancestors who have introduced fire, taught the preparation
of food, etc. The hero who is exalted as a leader in war belongs to a different
world a world faithfully mirrored in the heroic song or epic. As regards
their station in life, the heroes of Homer are still essentially tribal
chieftains, but the enlarged field of struggle, together with the magnified
characteristics which it develops, exalt the leader into a hero. With the
development of poetry, the forms of language also change, and become enriched.
The epic is followed by formative and dramatic art. All this is at the
same time closely bound up with the origin of the State, which now displaces
the more primitive tribal institutions of the preceding period. When this
occurs, different customs and cults emerge. With national heroes and with
States, national religions come into being; and, since these religions
no longer direct the attention merely to the immediate environment, to
the animal and plant world, but focus it primarily upon the heavens, there
is developed the idea of a higher and more perfect world. As the hero is
the ideal man, so the god becomes the ideal hero, and the celestial world,
the ideally magnified terrestrial world.
This era of heroes and gods is finally succeeded by a fourth
period. A national State and a national religion do not represent the permanent
limits of human striving. National affiliations broaden into humanistic
associations. Thus there begins a development in which we of the present
still participate; it cannot, therefore, be referred to otherwise than
as an age that is coming to be. We may speak merely of an advance toward
humanity, not of a development of humanity. This advance, however,
begins immediately with [p. 10] the fall of the barriers that divide peoples,
particularly with regard to their religious views. For this reason, it
is particularly the transcendence of the more restricted folk circle on
the part of religions that constitutes one of the most significant events
of mental history. The national religions -- or, as they are generally,
though misleadingly, called, the natural religions -- of the great peoples
of antiquity begin to pass beyond their original bounds and to become religions
of humanity. There are three such world religions -- Christianity, Islamism,
and Buddhism -- each of them adapted in character and history to a particular
part of mankind. This appears most clearly in the contrast between Christianity
and Buddhism, similar as they are in their endeavour to be world religions.
The striving to become a world religion, however, is also a symptomatic
mental phenomenon, paralleled externally by the extension of national States
beyond the original limits set for them by the tribal unit. Corresponding
to this expansion, we find those reciprocal influences of cultural peoples
in economic life, as well as in custom, art, and science, which give to
human society its composite character, representing a combination of national
with universally human elements. Hellenism and the Roman Empire afford
the first and, for Occidental mental development, the most important manifestations
of these phenomena. How immense is the chasm between the secret barter
of primitive man who steals out of the primeval forest by night and lays
down his captured game to exchange it, unseen by his neighbours, for implements
and objects of adornment, and the commerce of an age when fleets traverse
the seas, and eventually ships course through the air, uniting the peoples
of all parts of the world into one great commercial community! We cannot
undertake to delineate all aspects of this development, for the latter
includes the entire history of mankind. Our concern is merely to indicate
the outstanding psychological factors fundamental to 'the progression of
the later from that which was original, of the more perfect from the primitive,
partly under the pressure of external conditions of life and partly as
a result of man's own creative power.
Elements of Folk Psychology